Tuesday, September 18, 2007

4, Option 1: Deception Experiment

For my deception experiment, I spoke to another COMM 245 student, Zeyu, in two different mediums: Face-to-Face and online instant messaging (AIM). I decided to tell the truth in FTF, about my trip several years ago to Italy. He correctly guessed I was telling the truth because of several factors: I kept the conversation fluent, which he thought would eliminate any time needed for equivocation. I included lots of numbers and offered to provide proof by saying I have pictures and videos of my time in Italy. The truth bias played a large part in Zeyu believing my FTF vacation story: we tend to trust others and believe they are telling the truth. Since we were in a rich medium, bias was towards telling the truth. In addition, Zeyu told me that my story seemed very varied and complicated and not a simple story that could easily be made up. The FTF medium definitely affected my strategies for telling the truth: I tried to make eye contact as much as possible and not make extended pauses during my storytelling. I answered his questions quickly and continued telling my story.

I decided to describe a fictionalized vacation to Washington DC over last summer in my CMC deception experiment. I had used common details about such well known Washington DC tourist attractions, especially my visits to the White House, the US Mint, and Georgetown University. To further attempt to deceive in the CMC space, I did some research about tourist attractions on the internet prior to my instant message conversation and pretended I learned many of those new facts while on vacation. With all these factual details, I thought I would certainly deceive Zeyu. I wanted to be fairly specific with those details, but not too specific that it was suspicious. According to the Media Richness theory, in a lean medium like instant messaging, I can best selectively tell the most exciting details and think about how to answer questions before I respond to them. In the Zeyu thought my vacation story told through CMC was a lie because he realized the US mint and White House are no longer completely open to tourists anymore.



https://www.blogger.com/comment.g?blogID=5400576841210402935&postID=8673218457356537838

https://www.blogger.com/comment.g?blogID=5400576841210402935&postID=4805353059940773370

Assignement #4 FACEBOOK :)

This week I have put some thought and work into taking a closer look at the creation of a facebook profile. I have interviewed my roommate, Leah, and asked her about what she has chosen to put up and why. For starters, Leah feels like she has got a fairly standard facebook profile for a female college student. Her favorite part of facebook is looking at people's tagged pictures. I feel like this is an assessment signal because a photo of yourself is not really something you can lie easily about. If an image is altered it is generally very obvious, in addition there are many photos of Leah tagged of her so if she tagged herself as someone else in one it would be clear from the others that it was not actually her. The photos on facebook also tell a lot about a person but Leah said that after thinking about it for a minute maybe her pictures gave off a false impression of her life because most of her pictures are taken at night when she goes out with friends and to parties. So it may come across that she is a crazy partier all the time when in fact she has many other dimensions of her life that are not shown in her pictures. I agree with this and think that this is a common situation on facebook, so being familiar with the psychological space I think I take it into account that most people don’t show all aspects of their life in their pictures but mainly their party mode. This goes along with the hyperpersonal model and the over-attribution process because on facebook you don’t have a lot of cues to take from but pictures is one of the main cues and because people have less to look at but a lot of pictures you judge a person by their pictures and get an exaggerated impression of their partying as shown with Leah's profile.
Next, Leah looks at her wall as the second most important part of facebook. Her wall is a place where people will write messages to her that everyone can see. This is a conventional assignment in my opinion because generally the wall is not taken as a place to write serious messages but more of jokes or references to things that have happened, and often people write things that aren't true just as a joke. Therefore this is a low cost display that is changed often when it is replaced by new posts and isn't held accountable by most people. This goes along with the hyperpersonal model as well because of selective self-presentation. A wall is a place where people can write anything they want and then the owner of the page can choose to delete or keep the message therefore, deciding how they would like to present themselves. Facebook pictures are also an example of selective self-presentation because these pictures too have the option of being kept as tagged or untagged if the owner does not like how the message portrays them. Additionally, the wall posts can be seen as re-allocation of cognitive resources because Leah explained to me that a lot of times when she’s just browsing facebook she will go and look on other people’s walls and see what people have posted. Because it's not her wall she won't necessarily know what all the posts are talking about but with re-allocation of cognitive resources she will analyze the post and see if she can figure out what it could be referring too and this is definitely a close analysis of the text, including reading between the lines.
I next asked Leah, about the messaging aspect of facebook because there is another way to message people other than publicly on their wall and that is by sending them an individual message as if they have a mail box. This is a conventional assessment because the messaging is a lean environment and therefore being a deceptive media. She said that she doesn’t use this feature very often but the person she uses it the most with is her cousin Alek who is the same age as her. She said it's been really nice in college having facebook to talk to Alek because when they were younger they lived very far apart and she really only talked to him on holidays over the phone. Now that they can message each other she says it's a nice way that they can talk without having to devote a lot of time on the phone and she has really kept in touch with him more. This shows that in the hyperpersonal model the developmental aspect is really true. That people can get to know each other over CMC like Leah and Alek have become closer over time now that they are in college. The whole idea is that in CMC there is a time delay so it does take longer for people to get to know each other better in a CMC environment because of the fewer cues but the relationship will develop eventually.
Lastly, Leah talked about how when she meets someone briefly either in a class or at a party she likes to go back on facebook later on and look that persons profile up so that she can get a better idea of what they are like. Due to the fact that I do not think facebook profiles truly show all aspects of a person’s life and can exaggerate some aspects I do not feel like Leah is getting an accurate impression of others lives. This is like behavioral confirmation because when Leah looks at their profile she often sees the person in high party mode and then will take that exaggerated cue impression and when she may see them next, think and treat them as the high life partier that their profile portrayed them as.

Assignment 4: Option 2

Although Facebook profiles are becoming diverse in their complexity and depth with the introduction of third party applications, there remains the standard upon which the basic profiles exists:

* Portrait
* Status
* Personal Info
* Contact Info
* Interests / Favorites Info
* Education / Work
* The Wall
* Photos
* Friends
* Groups

Of these I would suggest that only the Portrait and Photos sections can be categorized as assessment signals, because they directly relate information about the user's appearance and often actions in candid shots. Whereas the rest can be described as conventional signals, because they use text to relate information without any substansive support. E.g. one can say they like listening to Britney Spears; maybe so, but you can see that the person has blonde hair in their photo.

After asking a friend to rate the accuracy of his profile elements, this was the response I recieved:

5 Portrait
X Status
4 Personal Info
4 Contact Info
3 Interests / Favorites Info
4 Education / Work
5 The Wall
4 Photos
4 Friends
4 Groups

Reviewing the results, we see that the user lied frequently but only slightly. The majority of his answers were a 4. I believe this falls in line with the theory that online impression managers will attempt to appear honest by lying but only slightly. In fact, I'm very sure the reason some of his profile is a bit misleading is his fear a job interviewer or professional colleague will see it. This follows closely with Walther's 1996 study that showed anticipated future interaction was a strong motivator for self-presentation maniuplation. This individual is afraid a professional peer will see the profile and think down upon him or not hire him. Therefore, while he tries to present himself "mostly truthfully" he likely lies by ommission more than by changing the accuracy of information.

However, upon analysis of the answers provided and his profile show that in fact a lot of the material is just dated or erronenous not of his fault. For instance, his address is wrong because he moved before the school year started, his education / work information is incorrect because he quit a job over the weekend, and his personal info incorrectly lists him as a Cornell alum because he was supposed to graduate last year but did not and Facebook doesn't have an easy remedy for this. Still there are some things I know to be purposefully dishonest in his profile such as length of employment at certain jobs (he wrote that he worked there longer). Also, I imagine he rates his photo section as innaccurate because I know he has untagged himself from certain pictures. Additionally, he is enrolled in certain Facebook groups of which he has no interest such a political group when he doesn't even vote or a band fan group that he has only listened to a couple songs but has a friend in.

4(1): Math Team = Must have been to Boston?

I am not a good liar (I would give out too many non-verbal and physiological cues), and I know it. So I decided to use a lean media, AIM, to lie. I chose my friend, H, with whom to conduct the experiment because she was the only friend from Cornell who was online on Sunday 12:45AM.
I told her beforehand that I am conducting an experiment for a psych class, which requires me to talk about my traveling experiences. I didn’t specify the purpose of the experiment, just to throw her off a bit.
I planned out what I would say for the lie before I contacted her, so it would seem more realistic. As a Chinese guy, it’s not really surprising if I said I have been to Shanghai, one of the most famous cities in China, but first, to see if she knows about the place better than I do, I asked her if she has ever been to there. Luckily, she has only been to Beijing. So I used the fragments of knowledge I acquired from a TV show about Shanghai a few years back and improvised a bit:
Me (12:52:03 AM): well, I must say that the food there is awesome
Me (12:52:15 AM): love meat buns
Me (12:52:33 AM): even tho I burned myself while eating it in the process
Me (12:53:51 AM): oh yeah, they had this underwater tunnels
H (12:53:56 AM): i dunno, touristy things you did there?
H (12:53:57 AM): XD
H (12:54:19 AM): for what, transportaiton?
Me (12:54:24 AM): kinda
Me (12:54:42 AM): but it's like a information/movie on the railed car thing
Me (12:54:55 AM): they tell you about history of shanghai and stuff
Me (12:55:05 AM): kinda like an underwater museum).
And I made it seem that it’s been a couple of years since I traveled there to make up for the lack of coherent chain of events. I made a few analogies between Shanghai and Beijing so it would seem more realistic for her (it was an easy task since I was born in Beijing), but we sidetracked a lot because of this – she was pretty excited to talk about her experience in Beijing.
After we concluded the first part of the experiment, we agreed to do the second part the next day (the same day considering it was 1AM in the morning) over brunch.
Anyways, we met up in Appel and started the second halve. For this part, I decided to talk about my trip to Boston, Massachusetts with the Math Team during the HMMT competition. I talked about my impressions on MIT and Harvard campus – Harvard seems more alive in comparison. Then, I went into some interesting details: on our way to a food court, we tried to find a bathroom but most of the restaurants were either closed or have no available bathroom; one guy complained that his groin hurts in the cold weather; the same guy proposed that we should urinate in public, etc.
Afterward, I told her about the purpose of the experiment and that I was lying in one instance. She was surprised at first but picked the correct one in the end. Her reason was that she knows that I was in the Math Team in high school so that one must be correct, so if it’s one truth and one false then the Shanghai one must be a lie. But after I told her that there are many competitions in the Math Team and that I could have been to anywhere, she started to waver and eventually reversed her answer. Later, she told me that she thought that I was telling the truth in both instances before I told her about the lying part.
I chose AIM to lie because I felt uncomfortable about lying. This corresponds with the Social Distance Theory which states that lying is uncomfortable and therefore people tend to use the most “socially distant” media to lie. However, I chose AIM over email (a even more socially distant media) because I wanted feedback so that I can see if she is buying my lies and I can adjust as I go. This follows the Feature Based Model as I needed the simultaneity of AIM to see my friend’s reactions to my lie and I needed the relatively long pause allowed in Instant Messaging to think up my next steps. Also, AIM is a distributed media. Though I wasn’t really concerned about the recordless part since I told her that I was lying afterward.
My friend wasn’t able to detect the lie because she was relying on verbal information in both instances, and contrary to the hypothesis discussed in class, her truth bias did not decrease in CMC.

Comments:
https://www.blogger.com/comment.g?blogID=5400576841210402935&postID=6678409767862076576
https://www.blogger.com/comment.g?blogID=5400576841210402935&postID=2141654875358656035

Assignment #4- The Truth Unfolds or Does it?

For this assignment I chose option 1, which was to deceive a friend in a rich and a lean medium. For my rich medium of choice, I decided to talk to my good friend from high school who attends school here with me. I sat her down and told her an experience I had during my study abroad semester in Australia.I decided this would be my opportunity to experiment with my acting skills, so I told the biggest lie about a make- believe adventure that I had going to the outback. In my story, I went to the outback for a week and was bale to ride a kangaroo through the desert. AL though the views were spectacular, the trip turned out to be a disaster because the people who I were traveling with got lost and we had to sleep in the car for a couple of nights. During the narration, I tried my best not to exaggerate the lie, but I continued to make it up while I went along. I then went to explain how there were wild camels, sheep, and horses in the outback and the only regret I had was not being able to stay in an aboriginal town. Surprisingly she took my story with awe and amazement, not having a clue that it was a lie. I really would of thought that she picked up on it when I mentioned that I rode kangaroos, but I guess that since she did not know much about Australia,s he took the information with an open mind of anything is possible. Despite the FtF interaction, she did not pick up on my nonverbal cues of mot directly looking her in her eye.
I told the truth to my friend online through AIM. I gave her an insight on an actual experience I had down under about how I went surfing for the first time but miserably failed. It was a funny story that she enjoyed without the digital deception. She did automatically ask me if it did really occur. I honestly found that weird but I guess since she is prone to people making up stuff through CMC, she had to double check. My FtF experience definitely confirms the deception detection theory in which people have a harder time telling whether or not the truth is being spoken to them.

Comments:
https://www.blogger.com/comment.g?blogID=5400576841210402935&postID=6110879681980286898
https://www.blogger.com/comment.g?blogID=5400576841210402935&postID=1790830434729753632

4. Facebook Lies

Many people use their online profiles to create impressions of themselves that are often more idealistic versions of themselves than their true selves. Take my friend, whom I will call John. John uses his Facebook profile to transmit information about himself that is often highly selective in order to create a favorable impression on those who see his profile. I asked him to rate the truthfulness behind what is said on his profile, following Catalina’s study. The major assessment signals, such as where he went to school, his gender, and birthday were all accurate, however, he says (on his profile) that his hometown is New York City, whereas the actual town is just outside the city, called Jericho. He says that most people have never heard of Jericho, NY, so he just says that he is from New York City. He claims that this is for simplicity’s sake, however there is a certain status associated with living in New York City that perhaps he wanted to be associated with, so he included it in his profile, even though it is not entirely true.

He was much more creative with his deception in terms of conventional signals. When I asked him about how truthful his activities and interests are, he indicated that on a scale of 1-5, these sections were probably a 3 in terms of truthfulness. He says that many of these activities were things that he was active in during his freshman or sophomore years, but not so much anymore. I asked him why he keeps them on his profile, and he said that it is a mix between being too lazy to take them off and because he still wants the status or prestige associated with those activities, even though he is not actively involved in them. He indicated that the items listed in the Music and Movie sections of his profile were certainly things that he enjoyed, but he said that he held back on a lot of bands and films that he really liked a lot more than some others he listed, but that aren’t considered “cool” or “Indie enough” so he elected not to include these things. When I asked him to provide some examples, he responded with “no further comment.”

The magnitude of John’s lies were quite small in that they were mostly half-true and only slightly enhanced the impression he gave off. However, the frequency of these small lies was much greater – he seemed to lie at least once in each of the sections that included conventional signals in his profile. He would probably not lie to a person face to face about things like his interests, activities, favorite music and movies, but it seems that Depaulo’s Social Distance Theory is relevant in describing this case in that lying is uncomfortable – he would not lie about these things in F2F interaction, but the social distance associated with online profiles and the difficulty of verifying the truth behind this kind of information makes it easier for him to pass on something that is not entirely true in order to enhance the impression he gives off through his profile. This is also described using the Identity-Based Digital Deception Model, as the information he includes in his profile is intentionally controlled, technologically mediated, and it creates a false impression in people who look at his profile and believe that he is truly associated with all of the things that he lists. John seemed to follow what Goffman and Baumeister described as Self-Presentation Goals – he attempted to appear attractive by presenting false or half-true facts about his life that increased his status or prestige. Also, he attempted to appear honest in that he only included things that he could actually talk about if someone asked him, based on having some association, although distant, to the activity/interest/music/movie that he included in his profile, thereby anticipating future interaction, as Goffman and Baumeister describe.

Comment 1
Comment 2

4. Facebook For College: A League of Its Own

&When I started this assignment I’ll admit that I did not expect to come across any digital deception because I couldn’t think of any friends who present inaccurate information in their Facebook profiles. Facebook is a widely used social networking platform that could easily be used for deception, however, it wasn’t until the platform’s recent transition from an exclusive application with only verified users to a program that anyone could use that I believe it became a valid tool for deception. I am a Facebook user within a college network which requires validation, this is the first level of protection against deception that is built into Facebook as it ensures that at least one piece of a user’s profile is accurate. Beyond that, within a verified network on Facebook, most social links are between real life friends or acquaintances, so deception becomes even less common because there is a social check and balances system in place. Although cases of deception are not uncommon within such verified networks, I believe that they occur with a much lower frequency than within large unverified networks like online dating services and other, more open, social networking web sites. It was with this idea already in my mind that I questioned my friend on the accuracy of his Facebook profile.

Prior to going through any specific information in his profile, my friend stated that all of the details were completely true. Sure enough, when we went through it piece by piece he claimed that each item was entirely accurate. Having lived with him for an entire school year, I went back through his profile and agreed with his evaluation. Beyond my concurrence, his friend was present while we did this assessment and she also verified each claim that he made. I will admit that profile information is rarely one hundred percent accurate, and my friend is a rather honest guy, but I also believe that many inaccuracies in college Facebook profiles are not due to deception, but rather outdated information or humorous jokes among friends. The fact that our friends on Facebook know us in person limits us on the amount of deceptive information that we can present.

In the case of Facebook for college networks, I don’t believe the Social Distance Theory is accurate. The Facebook platform is not a particularly rich media: it is asynchronous, and text based; however the social connections made through Facebook are generally between people that already know each other in person. The Social Distance Theory would predict that such a lean media would produce social connections that are more distanced. I suspect that digital deception happens in much greater frequency in unverified networks on Facebook, as well as other social networking websites like Myspace, and online dating services like Match.com. However, when talking specifically about Facebook for college networks, I see a unique social medium that is not as prone to deception as the equally lean, aforementioned mediums.

comments:
#1
#2

4: Un-Masking Facebook

I asked my good friend, let’s call her Kate, to rank her Facebook profile elements on a scale of 1 (completely inaccurate) to 5 (completely accurate):

Activities: 4
Favorite music: 5
Favorite movies: 5
Favorite books: 5
Favorite quotes: 5
About me: 5


Unlike her absolutely accurate email address, school, name, relationship status, and gender that she lists on her profile, the signals listed above are low-cost conventional signals. She could easily have changed her interests and activities but would have had trouble lying about assessment signals like gender and email.

I asked her to elaborate on how often she does the activities listed as equal elements on her Facebook profile. Her response did match my all of expectations of her, and the activities she listed as equal activities did not have similar emphasis:


tennis: a month ago (how often- every few months)
listening to music: 5 minutes ago (how often- every day)
sleeping: last night (how often- every night)
bikeriding: a few weeks ago (how often- every few weeks)
cards: 6 months ago (how often- rarely)
wiffle ball: a month ago (how often- every summer)
swimming: 2 months ago (how often- rarely)
walking around on nice days: a week ago (how often- every week)


Looking at her photographs, I found a few of her playing poker. She is by no means a typical card player, though she lists it on her activities. One who doesn’t know her might get the impression that she is very into card games because of the combined effect of the photographs and her mention of “cards” on her list of activities.

The results of my experiment coincide with those of Catalina’s study. Lies were very subtle, and ratings ranged from 4-5. However, the results of the Facebook experiment are not as surprising as Catalina’s; people see and interact with many of their Facebook friends every day in person, whereas those on dating websites have usually not yet met in person. I would have expected people to lie much more often on dating websites. However, both instances make sense in light of the self-presentation goals of Goffman and Baumeister. In both cases there is the anticipation of meeting the profile viewer in real-life, which urges people to make their profiles more accurate and insert only subtle lies, which could end up having an exaggerated effect.

She also stated that she is more hesitant to add or change information rather than delete it altogether. Due to the recordability aspect of profiles discussed by Goffman and Baumeister, Kate would appear dishonest if she were to completely revise her activities or favorite music. But if she were to remove those items altogether, no harm would be done. Overall, the asynchronous medium of Facebook allows Kate to lie subtly and frequently, which supports Goffman and Baumeister’s theory.

Personally, I leave all the fields which require conventional signals blank, aside from being too lazy to keep updating my favorite movies and music, I feel that my personality would be skewed and exaggerated depending on what I emphasize (both knowingly and unknowingly). This notion touches on the reduced-cues aspect of the Goffman and Baumeister in that I am concerned that limited profile information would exaggerate inaccurate impressions of my personality.







comments:

https://www.blogger.com/comment.g?blogID=5400576841210402935&postID=5075181914644979109

https://www.blogger.com/comment.g?blogID=5400576841210402935&postID=840963439825515129


4: Looking at Facebook

When I first reviewed this assignment the first thing I did was examine my own profile. After examining my profile I found that I did not really have much to lie about because my own profile was pretty sparse. Except for my profile picture in which I am dressed up from a formal, I felt that due to the lack of information, it was fairly representative of me.

For this assignment I examined my friend’s facebook profile. To protect his identity I’ll refer to him by his initials RHR. RHR is interesting in the fact that he has two profiles. He has one that very well represents him. He updates it often so it is fairly up to date and he does not seem to have any interest in exaggerating the truth. When I had him rate each part he averaged a 4.9 overall. When I tried to grade it myself I came out to the same conclusion.

RHR’s second profile is one that he made a couple months ago as a joke. He wanted to see if he could make a profile to hit on girls with. He replaced a picture of himself with someone that looked taller and stronger. He then filled his profile with completely fake information including multiple varsity sports, volunteering at different charities, and anything else that he thought could help. RHR then started friending girls and messaging them. The beauty of facebook is that most people put up real pictures of themselves so he could see how they really looked (even if they did select flattering photos) and at a school of over 10,000, there are over 5,000 girls to choose from. Many of them accepted his friending and many messaged back and forth with him. In addition to all the random people he friended, he also friended 2 girls that he knew pretty well (without them knowing it was him) and a girl that had rejected him in the past. Only the two he knew accepted and he had a bit of fun lying to them before he got bored and stopped using that profile. RHR’s fake profile is still online but he does not use it anymore.

RHR was able to lie in part due to the social distance theory. He was not face to face with the people he was talking to and they had no way to trace the conversation back to him. The beauty of facebook is that since it is so closely linked to people in most cases, most people do not suspect anyone would lie and therefore do not try to perform the deception detection they would otherwise. Media richness theory also played a roll in him not having to lie face to face. The conversations he had were asynchronous and recordable but he has no reason to be concerned with the fact that they were recordable because he had no attatchment to the profile. With RHRs real profile he does not mess with people. His real contact information is on that including his screenname, his email, and his cell phone number. It is associated with him and even though he does not know all of his 200 friends very well, everything he writes is recordable and depending on who he is talking to, the distance changes. However online with such distance there are no repercussions for him for lying so RHR does it.

Assignment 4: I know when you're lying

While the Internet is generally thought of as an ideal place to engage in deceptive behavior, a Facebook profile may not be the best place to test that out. On Facebook, many, if not most or all, of the people who look at your profile know you to some extent. Anyone can create a completely fake profile, but passing it off as real is nearly impossible unless the only people you friend are people who have never met you. What most people do is truthfully present their assessment signals, and tweak their conventional signals. It is debatable what would be considered assessment signals and what would be considered conventional signals on a Facebook profile. However, assuming that the people who look at you Facebook profile have spoken to you in person at least once, some characteristics that would fall under assessment would include profile picture, sex, college and year, birth date, and contact information. These are things that people would know having seen you or be able to easily verify. Assessment signals are more costly to display and thereby harder to fake. They are, as stated in the Hancock Digital Deception article, “links to a person’s ‘real world’ identity.” Conventional signals, on the other hand, are easy to manipulate. Most aspects of a Facebook profile fall under this category, including any of the information under personal information, groups, picture albums, and tagged pictures.

I asked my friend “John” to rate his level of honesty in his Facebook profile following the Catalina model, 1 for least accurate, 5 for most accurate. I noticed that just about everything he rated as 5 or a 1, which was usually a result of a joke, I agreed with. However, the aspects that he rated 3’s or 4’s I had slightly different opinions in that I thought he was less truthful. For example, for his Favorite Music section, he ranked himself a 4, showing that he didn’t believe it was completely accurate, but was for the most part. However, I know for a fact that he listens to a lot of mainstream rap and hip-hop, none of which was included in his list. He did include a lot of underground bands and classic artists, a few of which I’ve heard him mention but most of them never connected him to. I feel he probably included a number of these to make himself seem like less of a conformer because he didn’t want to admit how much he liked Kanye West or Timbaland to the entire Internet. However, he feels very comfortable admitting that to me in person. Also, if you look at “John’s” groups, you would see that he is in both the Cornell Democrats and Cornell Republicans groups, a move that obviously has some level of dishonesty in it.

My observations are in line with the Hyperpersonal and Social Distance Theories. “John” engaged in a lot of selective self-presentation in describing his personal info, what groups he chose to be in, and what pictures he chose to have of himself, exaggerating a lot of the information he wanted to be more public, thereby supporting the Hyperpersonal theory. In addition, he seemed fairly comfortable being dishonest and misleading about a number of things on his profile that he wouldn’t have necessarily said in person. This is in line with the Social Distance Theory which says that people who are uncomfortable lying in person feel more comfortable doing so over leaner, online media because there are fewer cues. Overall, “John’s” profile was fairly honest, but the things he lied about were all conventional signals that he would not have typically lied about in face-to-face interaction.

https://www.blogger.com/comment.g?blogID=5400576841210402935&postID=3289176100669916187

https://www.blogger.com/comment.g?blogID=5400576841210402935&postID=3289176100669916187

4 “Believe only half of what you see…”

The word deception is associated with negative connotations. We like to believe that it is reserved for cheaters, thieves, criminals, and the like; but as we delve deeper and deeper into the abyss of the online world and computer mediated communication, we find ourselves taking part in deception, intentionally or not. Online profiles, such as Facebook, are psychological spaces that can lead to identity based digital deception, where the sender, through a technology medium (i.e. the internet), intentionally instills false information and beliefs on the receiver(s).


In order to determine the validity of the information posted on these profiles, I interviewed a friend about the information posted on her Facebook profile. The sections were broken down into general info, contact info, personal info, photos, and groups. After having her go through her profile and rate the accuracy of her information on a scale of 1 to 5 (1 being the least accurate and 5 being the most accurate) she said that she was roughly a 5 for all the categories. However, knowing her very well I was able to decipher what was fallacious and what was factual.


I started with the conventional signals – those that aren’t very costly and are mainly identity based – because those were more prevalent. She listed various activities that made her seem very community service oriented; however, I know that she has only participated in such a cause (Habitat for Humanity) once. She also added that she is a huge fan of her college’s athletics, and I know that she is not a fanatic. So, in this case I would say that she is using deception to be more attractive to her fellow classmates by having the same interests. . She omitted her birth year, so her age would be questionable to someone who didn’t know her, and her relationship status (“complicated”) was also deceiving because she is not in a relationship – lying about age and relationship status don’t follow Catalina’s findings. In terms of my friend’s one assessment signal (her university issued email address), she was accurate.


According to Goffman and Baumeister’s self-presentation goals, it is easy to manipulate information on an online profile because it is editable, asynchronous, and has reduced cues. We use deception to appear attractive, by posting a particular picture or broadcasting a particular interest, and to appear honest, by posting charities and community service participation (because those are associated with positive qualities). These two aspects go along with the Hyperpersonal model, in which you selectively self-present those qualities or traits most favorable, portraying a more “ought” or “ideal” self rather than the “actual” self. My friend lied frequently about her “attractiveness” by broadcasting her causes on her page (in her activities, groups, etc.) and by posting about her “favorite team” most likely to fit in with her schoolmates. She also lied subtly by just saying “too many to list” by her music and books sections. These finding closely follow Catalina’s study, except the relationship status and age were not as accurate.


-----------------------------

Comments:

http://comm245brown.blogspot.com/2007/09/4-little-blue-lies.html

http://comm245brown.blogspot.com/2007/09/assignment-4-option-2-fakebook.html



4 Relying on "truth bias"

Because of this assignment, I was forced to lie to one of my friends through a lean media. I decided to lie to my friend through AIM because stopping to think on a near-synchronous media is more convincing than pausing in the middle of a conversation in a synchronous media, such as the phone. I did not want the interaction to be asynchronous (email, Facebook posts, etc.) because this would not allow me to answer questions or play off his reactions.

I decided to deceive my friend by saying that I traveled to Salt Lake City. I picked this area because not that many people know about all the sites that this city holds, and I had to do a research project on this city, which was to basically planning a trip to another state (I was unfortunately assigned to plan a trip to Utah). By picking this city, I was very knowledgeable about all the minor details from the costs of transportations to historical meanings of the major landmarks. By picking AIM, I knew I had to answer questions, so getting the story straight was a major strategy for me in convincing my friend, since I know I am not good at making up stories on my feet.

After this, I met with this person to tell him a traveling experience that actually occurred. I told them about a time that I went to China with my family to visit relatives. This was no problem at all because it actually happened. After I finished telling my story, I asked him if he could tell if I was lying or not.

He responded by saying that he did not think that I was lying on either occasion. I told him that I was lying in one of the stories, and he still had to think about it, but in the end he chose the correct one. He explained his thought process as he was deciding. He said that he could tell if I were lying to him in a real-time conversation by relying on facial expressions, tone of voice, hesitations, sweatiness, lack of eye contact, and stuttering. He also said that he could tell if I were lying online if I were lying on AIM if I paused for long periods. He also noted that I did not do much of any of these, which explains why he did not think I was lying the first time I asked. However, when I asked him why he ultimately decided that I was lying on AIM, he replied by saying that my descriptions did not describe personal experiences enough, while my descriptions on my other trip seemed sincere.

My friend relied on 3 methods of deception detection: verbal, non-verbal, and physiological. Only the verbal aspect was present in the leaner media, AIM. These methods are often not very accurate. It was not that he was able to pick up any of these during the interaction, since he thought that I was not lying in either occasion. However, when I told him I was lying, he decided that I was lying in the leaner media over the richer media because he did not think my experiences were personal enough. This incident heavily supports Bos et al’s research. He showed that people develop trust over FtF interactions much faster than any CMC environment. My friend ultimately relied on the truth bias, although I am pretty sure he was not aware of this, to decide which interaction was deceptive.


EDIT, Comments:
http://comm245brown.blogspot.com/2007/09/although-facebook-profiles-are-becoming.html

http://comm245brown.blogspot.com/2007/09/4-option-1-deception-experiment.html

4: Luke, I am your father

While talking to my friend C on aim, I attempted to deceive her with a false travel story that I had said occurred over the summer. I used a story I had heard from another friend, which went as follows: While in a fast food establishment standing in line to order lunch with a bunch of friends from high school, a man in line kept staring at one of my friends “R.” I described my friend “R” as rash and confrontational. After becoming uncomfortable, my friend R approached this man and asked him if he had a problem, to which he responded that he was sorry for staring, but looked just like a daughter he had lost. R was embarrassed, said she was sorry, and asked if there was anything she could do. The man replied, “Could you wave to me and say, ‘bye daddy’ when I leave the store?” R found this a strange request, but feeling the need to make up for her earlier behavior, agreed and did as he asked when he left the store. When it was her turn to order, her bill came to around $35, which is exorbitant in a fast food place for one person. When she questioned this, the cashier replied, “Your dad said you would pay for him.” My friend C was spellbound by my story, continually interrupting to ask what happened next and constantly providing feedback like ‘oh man,’ ‘lol,’ and ‘then what happened?’, amongst others, in between sentence-long messages sent. Although C found it strange that I had neglected to tell her this story after seeing her nearly every day, she did not seem to otherwise doubt my sincerity. I use her feedback and interest in the happenings of the story to confirm that she had no idea she was being conned. Now, I’ve had to sit through the original teller of the story con others FtF, and I am barely ever able to keep a straight face while listening to some poor sap get fooled. I believe I was much more successful at lying in lean media than in rich media, where my facial expressions, blushing, giggling, averted eyes, altered voice etc. would have given me away. Due to the reduced physical cues, a fairly poor liar like myself was able to go by undetected. This experiment seems to support both the Social Distance Theory and the Feature Based Theory. The Social Distance theory’s main tenet is clear in my usual discomfort at lying FtF, and the ease with which I was able to lie online. Since I found lying uncomfortable, using a socially distributed medium not only put me at ease, but reduced her suspicions due to reduced social cues. However, due to the synchronicity of the medium, I was able to answer C’s questions and react to her feedback, making the story seem to develop more spontaneously and appear more plausible. Like the Social Distance Theory, the distribution between C and I contributed to the success of my lie. Since I told C afterwards that I was lying, recordlessness was not so much of an issue since I was not concerned with being caught.
Less interestingly, during my true story about an instance of my summer vacation told face to face about a pretty hectic babysitting experience, C, although more skeptical of everything I said due to my recent lie and the fact that my FtF story was told after my lean media lie, eventually believed me since she is familiar enough with my mannerisms and expressions to know when I’m telling the truth or not. Plus, she knows enough about my life to be able to confirm the identities of people in the story and the place. I think this element of being familiar with the person with whom you are interacting has a huge impact on the ability to detect FtF lies, and to a lesser extent, CMC lies. Due to this factor, I do not believe the Media Richness theory, which indicates lying occurs more frequently in rich media, would occur very successfully between good friends.

4: Facebook Breakdown

This semester has been giving me several opportunities to, shall we say, study the art that is Facebook. So of course, I eagerly chose the option to deduce the anatomy of a Facebook profile. My method was simple: after signing on iChat and initiating conversation with one of my best friends, I asked her if I could interview her on the accuracy of her Facebook profile. Within seconds she complied and I gave her somewhat of a rubric in order to complete my survey. Scoring each element from 1 to 5 (1 being a blatant lie, 5 being completely honest), my friend rated the different aspects of her Facebook profile. I then compared her assessment, to my own opinion of how accurately her Facebook reflects her true self.

Overall, my friend had rated her Facebook at 3.6. I consider this to be relatively low since she doesn't blatantly lie at all on her profile (believe me, I'd know). However, upon making my own assessment of her Facebook profile, it became incredibly clear that my friend was cultivating a specific image of herself with the various conventional signals expressed through Facebook. Rather than focusing on her wide range of hobbies, my friend's Facebook profile really hones in on the fact that she is somewhat of a hippie (her quotes are "namaste" as well as a spiritually charged message from Buddha). While the hippie characteristic is absolutely a part of my friend's persona, I would never venture to say it is her only notable quality. However, based on her profile, this certainly seems to be the case.

While she never actually expresses false information, my friend's selective self-presentation causes her profile to evade a very specific personality. This is fitting with the Hyperpersonal Model which includes the act of self-presentation as a way in which people appear to be an exaggerated version of what they are due to a lack of information. In my friend's case, the lack of information happened to be descriptions of non-hippy activities, music, etc.

Additionally, my friend's profile supports Catalina's findings that most people lie, but on a small scale in online personals. While my friend may have left out unfavorable information, or information which did not fit in with a certain persona, this selective-presentation is of small magnitude compared with the fact that she could have completely made up information on her profile. However, not keeping with Catalina's study, my friend actually rated herself fairly low on accuracy. So, at least she's sort of being honest... right?

My Comments:
http://comm245brown.blogspot.com/2007/09/4-newsfeed-from-fb-profiler.html
&
http://comm245brown.blogspot.com/2007/09/4-facebook-for-college-league-of-its.html

4 | Lying, or how I had a smashing time, cheers.

(Option 1) The construction of the best lie is based on the foundation of truth, built with bricks of monotony, and roofed with planks of exaggeration. Books, articles, self-help guides, and even university studies have been written and established in dedication of lying. (After all, what is a Theatre class other than to present the most convincing lie possible – that is to say, acting?) But why is deception so easy for some, yet so difficult others? To what extent can our body language betray our words? And, most importantly, since the average success rate of a lie-detection machine is only a bit above 50%, why do we still use it?


Those are all valid questions, and perhaps except the last one, I was able to answer all of them during my experience with intentional deception. I decided to give a lie during the Face to Face setting, a rich medium for communication. I claimed to have visited England in the summer of 2003 in order to see my grand-parents.


My partner was unsuccessful in identifying the lie and thus incorrectly detected the truth/deception in this case. In my deception we observed many adherences to the Media Richness Theory in my behavior. I took clear advantage of truth bias – I projected an air of confidence and answered questions briskly in order to avoid having to delve into excessive detail. My knowledge of the English society helped tremendously (as did Monty Python shows), so my ability to come up with arbitrary but believable anecdotes was, to say the least, quite adequate. I recognized that in a rich medium, there are many cues that are difficult to control – body language, tone of voice, reaction time, consistency of detail, etc. In order to avoid the pitfall of “erring” and giving off excessive counter-signals, I chose a boring story – I did little in England aside from watching television shows in my grand-parents’ house. By claiming that I was not able to travel much, I instantly eliminated any potential questions regarding subjects that I’m not familiar with, e.g. How did the tubes work, what were Londoners like, etc. I also made sure I had my hands occupied at all times – either hold a cup of water or a piece of food/utensil; this way my hands are less likely to twitch, tap the table, or exhibit any common behavior of discomfort and hesitancy, two glaring signs of lying.


In order to detect my lies, my partner tried to scrutinize my response time and body actions, two major factors that I was ready to deal with. He approached using the methods from Media Richness Theory (focusing on the inability of people to control many cues in FtF), but fell to the pitfalls of Social Distance Theory when he gradually fell under my deception when I failed to show any initially noticeable signs of lying. That is, he tried to be careful at first but lost focus when I put up a bulwark of simple, straightforward responses.


My next vacation story, a trip to Singapore when I was 8 years old, was told through Instant Messaging, a decidedly lean form of communication. Since the event took place 11 years ago, I had difficulties recalling many details. Again Media Richness Theory homes in on my behavior – I chose to tell the truth since it’s easier to modulate my responses to represent a believable truth. The asynchronous nature of Instant Messaging gave me the time needed to remember certain facts, such as the main attractions we saw and the general atmosphere of the country. I took my time to best phrase my answers so they sound less awkward, as it was challenging enough to share experiences that I can barely remember. As poor a choice as it was, the truth was still easier to tell than the lie, since I did not have to exaggerate with details and anecdotes, so the whole ordeal seemed rather plain and simple from my perspective.


My partner, however, detected the truth as a lie. The most obvious “sign” for him was that since the vacation was so long ago, I should not have been able to remember much. Also, since I did not respond as readily as I did in a FtF setting, he mistook my slowness for hesitancy in order to come up with lies even though I simply needed a bit more time to construct the response. A few brief hiatus and lack of strong anecdotes are supportive of the Social Distance Theory, so my partner decided that my story was fabricated. However, his method of deception detection was clearly befuddled with my unfortunate choice, so in this case there is a counter-balancing interaction between the Media Richness Theory and the Social Distance Theory – that is, even though I chose a lean medium because the truth happened a while ago, he took my choice as representative of my inability to lie in FtF.


After the analysis, I believe that our ability to deceive is highly situational. My enthusiasm about England helped me to throw out fake responses quickly, but I gave the “wrong” signals for my truth due to poor choice. Body language can be controlled but only with planning, so in a normal day to day situation we probably cannot hold back some behaviors. Lastly, my experience shows that sometimes cues can be taken in the entirely wrong and opposite way, so perhaps our reliance on lie-detection machine is partially for reasons of consistency – while body language and other physical behaviors can be easily faked or misconstrued, brain waves are comparably regular and dependable.


http://comm245brown.blogspot.com/2007/09/4-option-1-deception-experiment.html#comment-6155766050953580161

http://comm245brown.blogspot.com/2007/09/assignment-4-i-know-when-youre-lying.html#comment-7102227322098703829

4- Delving Into "Georgina's" Facebook Profile

This week, I chose to examine and assess someone’s Facebook profile, specifically my friend Georgina’s* profile (*name has been changed), to see how accurately she displayed herself. Georgina has been one of my best friends for five years now through high school and through college, so I would have to say that I know her extremely well.

There are many elements to the Facebook profile, as I’m sure you all know. In case you don’t though, here are most of the elements that I had Georgina examine on her profile: sex (gender), interested in (sexual preference), relationship status, looking for (friendship, relationship, random play), birthday, profile picture, contact information, interests, favorite music, favorite television shows, favorite movies, about me, education information, and actual friends listed. This is certainly a lengthy list, but Facebook is a complex web arena. Georgina ranked how accurate each element she displayed was on a scale from one to five, five being completely honest, one being a complete lie. To compare results in an efficient way, I decided to rank her profile on the same objectives before she was given the questionnaire. I then compared the two sets of data on her profile.

I found that for the most part, Georgina was honest. When I say honest, I mean pretty honest, but not completely. In my opinion, her profile would get somewhere between a three and a four overall. I believe she used selective self-presentation tactics to portray herself in a specific way than she is most of the time. For example, her profile picture is certainly of her, but it is of her striking a ridiculous pose, one which I have never seen her do (and I’m with her a lot) and is certainly atypical. Her relationship status is that she has a boyfriend. I know this is not exactly the case, as the two are on a break, yet it is on her Facebook profile for the world to see. Her interests include being the captain of a kickline squad, but her participation in that was over a year ago when she was in high school. Georgina’s friend count at Cornell is currently at 201. While she is an outgoing person, I am with her most weekends and know that she does not know this many people here. Other than these slight discrepancies, I would say that Georgina’s profile is mostly representative of her. I maintain that the information on her profile, along with everybody else’s, is put there to portray her in a certain light.

4: Little Blue Lies

Deception is a part of everyone’s daily life. In an average day, we deceive someone 1.96 times, and do it over just about any type of communication medium. Deception becomes especially important for networking and dating websites like facebook or match.com or anything else that has you create a personal profile. As we learned in class there is often a certain degree of deception involved in these types of profiles. Why would people put false information or bend the truth on these pages? Because they want to be liked. By changing the information displayed about yourself, you can manage the impressions others get of you and hopefully make them like you more.

On a facebook profile, almost all the information is in the form of conventional signals, with hardly any assessment signals. Basically everything someone chooses to display on your profile can be changed by the user as a form of impression management. Things that are assessment signals in ftf communication, like your physical appearance, can be easily changed in facebook. You just change your profile picture to whatever you want and untag/delete all the pictures you don’t want. Actually, the only piece of information in a facebook profile that can’t be easily changed to whatever you want is who you are friends with or in a relationship with (because they both require the other person to approve it as well). This means that you could easily create a completely fake profile. Naturally nobody does this, however, because nobody wants to get caught in a lie. Therefore, most digital deceptions in these profiles are small, and hard to prove.

For this assignment, I looked at one of my friend’s facebook profile and had him rate it as we learned in class. He gave himself a score of a 4.8, citing a little bit of fudging on the “Interests” and “Favorite Music” sections. I rated him as well and gave his profile a 4.7, finding deceptions in the same places he did. Basically, he tried to edit his personal information to highlight how artistic he is: writing, poetry, traveling, etc, while leaving out things I would have put on if I had written his profile: comics, warcraft…. A similar thing happened in the favorite music category. He listed Johnny Cash, Iron and Wine, and the Pixies on there, when I have only heard him listen to whiney emo tunes. Given that the rest of his profile was pretty accurate, I would say that neither the frequency nor the magnitude of deception in his profile were too big. Everybody wants to present themselves in a good manner, so I expect a certain degree of innocent fiction in people’s profiles. At the same time, nobody wants to get caught in a lie, so most deceptions tend to be very minor ones.

Assignment Four: Deception and Facebook Profiles

Practically all information in a Facebook profile is a “conventional signal,” which unlike an assessment signal is easy to manipulate at will. Although theoretically there is very little in an online profile that is impossible to manipulate, some information is clearly easier to control.

The name of the school that the student attends, personal photos, and the wall all are assessment signals. Taking “the wall” as an example, it is almost impossible to force others to write (or not write) on your wall in a particular manner, although one does have the ability to delete certain posts. In addition, it is unfeasible that somebody would lie about the school that they attend, as one must verify this information with a functional university email address.

In contrast, other aspects of the profile are easy to manipulate, and therefore are clearly conventional signals. Such components include interests, favorite movies, etc. This information is easily manipulated because it rarely can be independently verified.

In order to investigate deception online, I analyzed the profile of one of my friends. First, I had my friend reflect on how well he thinks that each component of his profile accurately reflects his true personality. The table below depicts his responses:

Political Views 5
Birthday 5
Residence 5
School Mailbox 5
Activities 5
Interests 4
Favorite Movies 4
Favorite Books 4
Favorite Quotes 5
“About Me” 2
Photos 4.5
Friends 5
Wall 5
Groups 4
Favorite Music 3
Mean 4.37
% Of Lies 47%


After looking at his responses, I myself independently analyzed the accuracy of his profile. I found that his self-report closely aligned with my independent investigation.

My experiment’s results conform to those of Catalina’s study. I too found that in keeping with Goffman and Baumeister's presentation goals, people lie frequently, but subtly in online profiles in order to balance the competing goals of both appearing attractive and honest. The fact that the communication channel is editable, asynchronous, and allows for only reduced cues, enabled my friend to lie frequently in order to appear more attractive to those reading his profile; this is evident by the fact that he lied 47% of the time.

This supports the Social Distance Theory’s prediction that people are likely to lie online due to the fact that lying makes people uncomfortable, and that this discomfort is alleviated by the reduced availability of cues in a mediated setting. This finding, in contrast, contradicts Hancock’s Feature Based Model, which predicts that people are more likely to lie while using communication channels that are recordless, synchronous, and distributed. Although Facebook is distributed, it is also asynchronous and recordable, which suggests that my friend would lie less frequently on Facebook than in a ftf situation (which is both synchronous and recordless). The fact that my friend lied an extremely high percentage of the time in his online profile, indicates that this is not true. Furthermore, the finding supports the Hyperpersonal Model’s expectation that people selectively self-present online in order to mold other’s impressions of them.

The fact that most of the lies were very subtle supports Catalina’s finding that the lies in online profiles are small in magnitude. Although he lied frequently, my friend wanted to appear honest and credible to those reading his information because of the fact that he interacts with many of them on a daily basis in ftf situations.

http://comm245brown.blogspot.com/2007/09/although-facebook-profiles-are-becoming.html

http://comm245brown.blogspot.com/2007/09/assignment-4-option-2-fakebook.html

Monday, September 17, 2007

4: the book of faces . . . OR LIES!!!!

I've gone through several personal opinions of facebook throughout college. When I made my first profile, I just tried to answer the questions accurately. Then I realized that almost every single friend I made in college would see my profile, so I edited it to make myself look better. Soon after that, I realized that I hated myself for being a conformist, and went back to writing what actually described me. When I look at anyone's profile, more than looking at their actual information, I tend to try and evaluate what they're trying to show me. Are they trying to look cooler than they are, more attractive, accurately present themselves, just goof around?

My friend, C, that I interviewed had similar opinions about facebook, but not nearly as strong as mine. He updated his profile often to try and keep it accurate. In accordance with that, he gave high accuracy ratings, fours and fives, to every element in his profile. In fact, the only specific thing he could point out as not being true was the inclusion of the band "Ok Go" in his music section, because he only had 9 of their songs.

I know him fairly well and I couldn't find anything in his profile that would be deceptive to a stranger. His profile picture is him dressed as a gangsta rapper of sorts, but it's pretty clear that it's a joke, so I don't count that as deceptive. Since I didn't find much deception, my friend's profile fits the Richness theory stating that lying is ambiguous and therefore requires a rich media. That's the only theory I see agreeing with this, even though I feel that he just isn't a big liar.

Although pretty much everything on facebook can be forged, there are some standard assessment signals like domain of email address (@cornell.edu...). Any picture can be used, so lying is almost effortless. Interests, movies, books, movies, religion, political views, and major are similarly fakeable.

I believe that there are more important conventional signals than favorite quotes. Some people may consider seeing "going for walks" in a profile's interests section meaningful, but I'm much more influenced by different signals. For instance, the size of somebody's profile can give you an idea of how much time they spend adding to and tweaking their info. If I see one of my friends updating their profile every day and they have 100 entries under their "favorite" books, I'm personally likely to believe a) that there are a lot of subtle lies and b) they are a little too into their online persona. On the other hand, somebody who lists a few interests, a few books, a few quotes, and doesn't describe themselves as "a chill person" in their "about me" will earn a gold star in my impression formation notebook. I would believe that they spend a reasonable (read: small) amount of time on facebook and they don't see it as the end all be all of their social life.

The main point I'm trying to make here is that there are lots of signals facebook users encounter that aren't as straightforward as "interests" or "favorite books". Most of these are probably conventional (I can make my profile ridiculously short to convince people I'm too cool for facebook) but I think that with these slightly more subconscious decisions, the true self tends to come out more.

Comments: http://comm245brown.blogspot.com/2007/09/4-looking-at-facebook.html
http://comm245brown.blogspot.com/2007/09/4-believe-only-half-of-what-you-see.html

4: Option 2- Sweet Little Facebook Lies

Examining Facebook profiles seems like a great way to extend the findings of Catalina’s study on online dating digital deception. I decided to dissect the Facebook profile of a friend I met during freshman year of college, about three years ago, who I now live with. In Hancock’s “Digital Deception” article, he discusses Donath’s description of assessment signals, “costly displays directly related to an organisms characteristics” and conventional signals “low-cost displays that are only conventionally associated with a characteristic” (291). I had my friend assess the accuracy of her responses to all the main features of her profile on a scale of 1 (completely false) to 5 (completely accurate) including assessment signals, such as her sex (5), birthday (5), college email address (5) and picture (3.5). While it may not always be the case in digital spaces, I consider these assessment signals in the context of Facebook. Since Facebook friends usually know each other in real life as well, it would be very difficult for her to escape the physical realities that she is female, in her 20’s, and has the physical features shown in her picture. Obviously it would be nearly impossible to fake the cornell.edu email address. She also rated her conventional signals including Political Views (5), Activities (3), Favorite Music (4), TV Shows (5), Movies (5), and Books (5).

Catalina discussed Goffman and Baumeister’s understanding of “Self-Presentation Goals”. Basically, people want to appear attractive so they prefer a media that is editable, asynchronous, and has reduced cues compared to FtF and all these factors which can lead to deception go along with Selective Self-Presentation (Facebook fits this very well). The balancing force against deception is that people also don’t want to seem dishonest. They consider that in the future they may meet people reading their profile, or those people they already know viewing their profile may get to know them better, and the selective presenters don’t want to be caught lying. Therefore, as Catalina pointed out, people will tend to lie frequently but also subtly.

My analysis of my friend’s profile seemed to fit these theories. She gave herself a 3 for activities because she had listed that she was an author in a student literary magazine, which she intended on contributing too, but never actually did. I would also say she had some subtle deception in this category since she listed herself as the president of the Red Cross Club, when I know she is actually co-president with another friend of ours. Both of these slight lies make her seem worthy of more admiration for being very involved in so many activities. For music she also didn’t give herself a perfect score, because it is hard to be all inclusive when listing a few bands or styles. Perhaps her most interesting rating was for her picture, which she gave a 3.5. While most people would argue that the picture is hard to lie with, she told me that she had taken several pictures of herself and then ‘photoshopped’ the best one to give herself more hair, and make her eyes greener. I would almost argue for an even lower rating, since I know she also wears color enhancing contacts to change her actually hazel eyes to appear very green. It is interesting that she considered this set of deceptions less ‘false’ than mentioning a literary magazine she wasn’t included in. All in all, we both thought each of her deceptions were subtle enough to go unnoticed by the average Facebook stalker.

Does our attention to facebook correlate with our self-esteem? -- Assignment 4

For the fourth assignment, I chose to do the second option, and used my brother as my subject. After a little bit of explaining, I got him to rate several different elements of his facebook profile on how they represented the "real-life him". Since I'd say I know him better than just about anyone, and he obviously didn't mind admitting any false representation, I felt that he was honest even to the point of self-deprecation. The elements I chose for this assignment were profile picture, relationship status, photos tagged of him, and groups and personal info.

For the profile picture, he first gave himself a 5, then changed his mind to a 4. Since the picture is of him and three of his frat brothers, and he is smiling in a bit of an affected manner, he felt that the picture was not entirely representative of the way he presents himself face-to-face. The next element I asked him about was relationship status. As others have previously insinuated, the relationship status aspect of facebook is one that is at once very significant and potentially deceptive. In my opinion, it is perhaps the most drama-inspiring element of the entire facebook profile page. When I asked my brother to rate its accuracy (he is listed as "single"), he laughed, then vacillated between 2 and 3, finally deciding on 3. At the moment, he is currently seeing a girl, but they aren't officially dating, hence the still "single" status. However, he wouldn't consider himself entirely single, but at the same time he doesn't want to be too forward and change the status before the appropriate amount of time has passed and the relationship is cemented.

Moving on to the next element, I inquired what he thought about the validity of his photos tagged of him. I chose this element because it is definitely one of the most looked-at elements on facebook. Perhaps for this reason, I came into the experiement thinking that people are most conscious of the photos tagged of them, and more inclined to "de-tag" pictures that they feel do not represent their ideal selves. In my brother's case, however, he gave himself a higher rating of 4. He said the only reason he didn't give a 5 was because the vast majority of the pictures displayed on facebook were taken when he was out with friends at night, and failed to represent the other aspects of his life. Next was facebook groups and personal info. I group the two together because he gave the same response for both elements: 3. His reasoning was that he does not often update either element and the last time he updated them was at least a year ago. Clearly, he has changed his interests and hobbies since then, and even his groups of friends.

In analyzing my brother's responses to my questions, I believe that he was just about dead-on, with the exception of his tagged photos. When he gave the accuracy of his photos a 4, he over-attributed the importance of his social life, forgetting that facebook only portrays how he acts in one situation. None of his photos, for example, showed him at any sports events or at home with his family and dog, all things that take a place of importance in his life.

When viewing my brother's facebook profile in light of his real-life self-presentation and the theories we have studied, I believe that my findings are not in conjunction with digital-deception theories. The majority of the deception taking place on my brother's facebook profile is simply due to his lack of effort in the upkeep of his online image, and therefore his actions were not deliberately deceptive. Therefore, I do not believe that one could say that the discrepancies between real life and my brother's online persona are due to any cognizant decision on his part to decieve any potential visitors to his profile. While one might at first point to the Social Distance Theory and say that he is distancing himself from others by not keeping his image up-to-date, I know that it is really due to a lack of motivation on his part to present the exact same persona online as in real-life. I think some of his openness can be accredited to his own self-confidence and his feeling that he really has no aspect of himself that he would like to hide. Instead, he presents only what is convenient for him to present given his busy schedule as a junior in college and his low level of interest in improving his image to those who see him more often online than in real life.

On this note, I think it would be interesting to draw a connection between people's satisfaction with their real selves and the amount of time they spend perfecting their ideal selves on facebook. Perhaps those with more fragile self-esteems feel the need to build themselves up superficially by spending more time in their ideal world, fine-tuning the people that they pretend to be every time they log-in to facebook. It would be an interesting study, to say the least.

4 Why I Should Never Play Poker

For this assignment, I chose to see whether my friend could determine the truthfulness of stories in both FTF and CMC contexts. I tried to tell two equally plausible travel stories, though only the one I told in person was true.
During my FTF encounter, I told my friend about my sixth grade trip to Disney World. Because I was aware that my story was being scrutinized for fabrications, my delivery probably had all the earmarks of a lie. My storytelling capacities improved over the course of the story, but my friend remained skeptical throughout. Lying on the internet proved easier than telling the truth in person. While conversing through instant messages, there was no need for me to conceal the telltale signs that characterize FTF deception.
My invented story consisted of a road trip with my parents to the Grand Canyon. I tried to include details which might persuade my friend to believe that I was conveying a rich, nuanced memory. Supposedly, my mom’s fear of heights resulted in us forgoing the donkey ride around the canyon and spending most of our time in the gift shop. Despite my best efforts, my friend correctly identified my trip to the Grand Canyon as fictitious. She recognized that a road trip to the Grand Canyon would be a substantial undertaking for a family from Long Island and also questioned my inability to “remember” which side of the canyon we visited.
In this case, Social Distance Theory explains my decision to choose instant messaging as a medium for deception. I thought there would be more room for error if I were physically removed from the person I was trying to deceive. On the other hand, the Feature-Based Approach predicts that instant messaging and FTF interactions will yield equivalent lie frequencies. Both types of interactions are synchronous, though instant messaging possesses this quality to a lesser extent. Instant messaging compensates by being distributed, unlike FTF situations, which have the advantage of recordlessness. For this assignment, recordlessness probably played a small role in considerations regarding which media to use in deception, since the lies were innocuous and part of a game-like exercise. Though my friend ultimately decided that my online story was the lie, she noted that she was skeptical of both stories for different reasons.
Online, the most salient tip-off was the content I presented, while nervous giggling caused her to question my face-to-face honesty. The dubious facts I presented online eventually outweighed my suspicious FTF affect in my friend’s decision. I tend to think that if both stories were equally plausible, however, the distribution inherent in instant messaging would have worked to my advantage and my friend would have believed the online story. When recordlessness is not crucial, instant messaging may prove superior to face-to-face interactions, as Social Distance Theory would predict.

comments:
http://comm245brown.blogspot.com/2007/09/4-option-1-deception-experiment.html

http://comm245brown.blogspot.com/2007/09/4-facebook-lies.html

I'm so much cooler online... Yeah, I'll see ya online - Assignment 4

I recently read somewhere that Facebook was offically launched in the year 2004. If this fact is true, it is stunning to see how quickly it has become one of the most popular forms of online communication in such a short period of time. Over the weekend, I interviewed a friend from home about her Facebook profile. Since I have known her for many years, she knew she had to be honest in rating the validity of each of her profile statements.

The first question I asked was about her relationship status. I know she has a boyfriend and has for quite some time, but she has chosen to keep this particular section of her profile blank. I asked her why she hasn't updated her "relationship status" and she came up with various answers such as: "I just haven't had the chance to", "it's nobody's business", and "he doesn't have himself listed as 'In a relationship', so why should I?". I found this quite interesting. Her boyfriend goes to school back home, which is about 6 hours away from her school. I wonder if both of them don't want to flaunt the fact that they are 'taken'? Interesting...

Next I moved on to her 'Personal Info'. Under 'Activities', my friend listed that she dances and plays soccer. She admitted that she has not played soccer for 3 years, and has not danced for 2 years. Next, I moved on to 'Interests'. Her interests included: "music, going to the beach, hanging out with friends, reading". She claimed that all of these were true, and gave them a 5 (even though I know she lives in Ottawa and probably only has the opportunity to go to a very poor quality beach about 5 times a year). She gave herself 4's on 'Favourite Music', 'Favourite TV shows' and 'Favourite Movies'. She admitted her 'Favourite Movies' were not her true favourites and mainly chose the most popular, award-winning movies. She did not list any "Favourite Books", which I found odd because one of her 'Interests' included "reading".

I then decided to scan through some of her "Groups" and other applications she might have on her profile. She had an NHL hockey application, and admitted to me that she didn't follow the NHL much at all but that since she was Canadian, she felt she should add it. She was also a member of a lot of Harry Potter groups, groups associated with her hometown and groups associated with her old high school. I found that her true interests and activities were shown particularly well through the groups she was involved in. I think the reason behind this is the fact that she truly wanted to learn and be associated with topics she was actually interested in. Also, most people don't "profile-stalk" by looking at one's Groups.

In analyzing every detail of my friend's profile. I realized how easy it is to manipulate how you are perceived online. In this specific profile, because of the big "NHL application", and the activities listed as "dance and soccer", I would assume she is very athletic. In reality, she is not very athletic and doesn't particularly follow or play sports very often. She also altered a few of the things she stated under her "personal info" in order to appear a certain way. My friend's deception stategies are very well carried out in her profile because they are extremely hard to detect. She discretely manipulated her profile, while maintain the appearance that she was being honest. The alteration of even the smallest piece of information, such as an addition of specific application, can change one's interpretation of a profile. As Brad Paisley puts it in his new song... "I'm so much cooler online!"

4 | Facebook Fun

Facebook profiles offer many layers of self-presentation and further convolute the game of deception. Although deception is defined as a strategic act that is part of an ongoing, interactive communication process by the Interpersonal Deception Theory, Facebook allows users to use deception as a medium for entertainment in addition to strategic deception.

To explore this new form of trivial deception, I turned to my friend Phil’s profile and asked him to rate the accuracy of his basic, personal, educational, and work information. All of the traits rated are conventional, as he provided the information. After reviewing his assessment, we both agreed that most of his information was completely accurate. I used his wall and photos as assessment signals, which his friends created for him. Phil’s friends present him as an outgoing, funny, and likable person. His assessment signals match his conventional signals by reinforcing his laid back interests in partying, sports, and playing the guitar.

Despite a strong congruence in Phil’s assessment and conventional signals, there were a few blatant lies in his conventional profile. Although Cornell is known for offering many majors, we both knew Beerpong Physics and Sexual Escapades were not his real majors. He also mentioned that he slept in the nude and worked at a money laundering pastry shop.

Although Phil lied through conventional signals, he did not intend to mislead anyone. His conventional signals help break the ice of mediated, online communication. He told me that its funny to see Beerpong Physics in a field that the rest of the world takes seriously; it reveals a strong sense of humor. He also told me he’s not ashamed of his major nor is trying to hide it; he just chose to represent himself this way for a good laugh. His behavior models a modified Social Distance Theory in that people are more prone to lie online over face to face. Phil lied to compensate for the mediated nature of Facebook profiles and add humor to his impersonal representation.

While Phil doesn’t believe he lied, he still misrepresented certain aspects of his profile to accomplish a goal. In traditional deception the goal is malignant, but in Phil’s case he utilized deception to achieve humor. Both forms of deception rely on conventional signals and illustrate how signal manipulation can affect representation in different contexts. Beerpong Physics and Sexual Escapades is not an isolated case. Many girls and a few boys list themselves as in relationships with other people of the same sex, despite the fact they are clearly heterosexual. These instances are analogous to Phil’s use of deception as a way to break the ice and utilize conventional signal misrepresentation in a novel, humorous way.

The variety in which people use deception make it difficult to classify a lie, which depends on its social context. This variability may help explain the inconsistencies between the Media Richness Theory and Social Distance Theory. Humorous deception follows the Social Distance Theory in that deception is utilized more in mediated environments to add humor. However, the Media Richness Theory focuses more on serious lies that are intended to deceive rather than placate, and should take place in a face to face medium. A stricter definition of deception may help bridge the gap in the theories, as deception occurs in many forms for unpredictable purposes.

Comment 1
Comment 2

4: Facebook Frenzy

I chose to write about option 2 this week since Facebook is the most popular social networking site for college students. Off the top of my head I can only think of one friend at Cornell who doesn’t have Facebook. (I know. How does she live?) Anyways, because so many people have Facebook I knew that I was bound to find someone that would be willing to rate their own profile.
I had my friend rate each of her elements one through five. I then also ranked them, based on what I know about her from our friendship thus far. According to her, she was very honest. She ranked her basic info, contact info, activities, interests, and TV shows as fives, and her music and movies as fours. The things that she felt she had manipulated were conventional signals. This is not uncommon as people are selectively self-presenting everytime they edit their Facebook profile. (And we all know when you’re editing your profile because Newsfeed so graciously tells us).
For the most part I agreed with the numbers my friend ranked. The main discrepancy I saw was that in her activities she has five things listed, three of which she did in high school and does not participate in at Cornell.
There were two other things I noticed about her profile, that I didn’t address when I orginially asked her to rate her profile. The first is the number of friends she has. As I write this she has 438 friends at Cornell. Now, she is a very social girl but I highly doubt that she is actually friends with half of them. Part of Facebook though is the number of friends you have- so skillfully displayed on the left side of the screen for all to see when they visit your page. The other part I noticed was her relationship status. She has been dating her boyfriend for a long period of time, but right now she has no relationship status. The funny thing about the relationship status, though, is that it gets changed on a regular basis. They seem to be “in a relationship” when they’re in a good mood/on good terms, but often have nothing at all if they’re fighting. This is a whole other issue of self-presentation.
Overall my friend’s profile is accurate; it is just the small things that may not be exactly true. It is hard to lie about assessment signals on Facebook, though, because there are plenty of pictures, and most people actually know you.